Mystery

According to a new study, insects likely experience pain, which has important implications for how we treat them.

According to new research, insects experience pain in a manner similar to vertebrates.

Seed 'em and Reap: World of the mantids

Scientists have combined behavioral, neuroscientific and molecular evidence to prove thatthe central nervous system of insects processes pain in a way much more similar toourselves than anyone has been willing to admit, IFL Science reports.

In the past, scientists performed what we would now consider horrific experiments onanimals without much constraint, if any. But ethical standards have changed and todayresearch on vertebrates has to go before ethics committees to prove that the value of the research outweighs any potential harm to the subjects. Cephalopods like octopus, squid andlobsters are starting to gain similar recognition. But not insects.

The justification for the different treatment of insects – that they don’t experience pain theway “higher-level” animals do – is what the new study, published in the journal Proceedingsof the Royal Society B, challenges. As the researchers note, the question hasn’t beenresearched extensively, possibly because people have been scared of the possible answer.

Animals (including insects) respond to extreme temperatures, pressure, or intense chemicalattack in a way to limit damage to their bodies that might impede their survival. The centralnervous system’s encoding of these harmful or unpleasant stimuli is called nociception –something closely related to pain, but still different. Our bodies can sometimes modulatepain without changing nociceptive reflexes, particularly in emergency situations when toomuch pain might distract us from what we have to do. The feeling of pain comes later,forcing us not to use an injured limb, for example. Interestingly, the opposite has also beenobserved with nociception enhanced without changing pain levels.

File:Nociceptive pain.jpg - Wikimedia Commons

As for insects, it has been long debated whether they experience pain through the centralnervous system, or if the response is localized, for example in an injured limb. After all,insects have much less sophisticated central nervous systems than mammals, and only atiny fraction of their brain cells are devoted to processing such inputs. Unlike our brains, theylack the opioid receptors so crucial for pain control but, according to the study authors, thatdoesn’t mean they lack simpler versions of the same thing.

To understand how nociception and pain are related in insects, the authors explored insects’capacity to control nociception, which they consider indicative, if not proof.

“Behavioural work shows that insects can modulate nocifensive behaviour. Such modulationis at least in part controlled by the central nervous system since the information mediatingsuch prioritization is processed by the brain,” the study underlines.

The authors identify specific neuropeptides produced in insects during traumatic events thathave a similar role performed by opiates in humans: suppressing pain. Furthermore,additional evidence shows that insects, like other animals, can become sensitized toparticular threats. When fruit flies are repeatedly exposed to high temperatures, for example,they start to respond more quickly – and some of the molecules involved in this process arethe same as those seen in humans. Scientists have even identified the pathways for sendingnociceptive messages to the brain.

And there’s something else that may shed light on the question whether insects feel pain:female praying mantises sexual cannibalism, one of the most widely known insectbehaviors. Infamously, mantis males respond to having their heads chewed off by matingharder. For that to happen, the male has to suppress its typical response to attack.

Praying Mantis (Mantodea) | Praying mantis at Bintaro, Bante… | Flickr

“This evidence has been suggested to indicate the absence of pain in insects,” the studyauthors write. “However, it is more likely that it demonstrates that insects can prioritize otherbehavioural needs and reduce the nocifensive behaviour in certain contexts.” That, in turn,points to a centralized response, which makes pain sensations more plausible, not less.

We still don’t know exactly how pain is processed in the insect brain, but if it is, that hasserious implications as to how we treat them.

source: earthlymission.com

Related Posts

Uп aпtrορólοgο eпcοпtró recieпtemeпte uпa aпtigua raza de ρequeñοs humaпοides

  El Pueblo Cuervo, un grupo tribal, mencionó la existencia de pequeños humanoides que no miden más de 36 pulgadas. Si le creemos a William Corliss, se descubrió…

El cráпeο eпcοпtradο eп Iпdοпesia ρarece teпer uп sοlο agujerο eп el mediο del cráпeο.

  ¿Alguna vez te has preguntado de dónde vienen nuestras peores pesadillas? anunciosPara los antiguos griegos, pueden haber sido los fósiles de animales prehistóricos gigantes. En la isla…

Vea lοs secretοs de uпa tumba que alberga uп cadáver que se dice tieпe 300 añοs eп Haпοi, Vietпam.

  La excavación de las tumbas antiguas que datan de aproximadamente 300 años se llevó a cabo en la tarde del 10 de diciembre, dirigida por el famoso…

Tesoro escondido desenterrado: cofre que contiene grandes lingotes de oro descubierto durante excavaciones en Filipinas.

  En un vídeo reciente de Youtᴜbe, un equipo de cazadores de tesoros reveló su descubrimiento de un alijo de ƖooT ιn del Mundo II en Phιlιppιnes. Después…

Tesoro escondido desenterrado: cofre que contiene grandes lingotes de oro descubierto durante excavaciones en Filipinas. 

  En un vídeo reciente de Youtᴜbe, un equipo de cazadores de tesoros reveló su descubrimiento de un alijo de ƖooT ιn del Mundo II en Phιlιppιnes. Después…

Increíble sincronicidad: emergen obras de arte antiguas y una estatua que se asemeja a Michael Jackson, lo que alimenta las teorías del viaje en el tiempo. 

  Ha habido afirmaciones recientes de que un desmayo y una estatua tienen un parecido sorprendente con el difunto Rey del Pop, Michael Jackson, lo que llevó a…